Multidimensional intuitive-analytic thinking style and its relation to moral concerns, epistemically suspect beliefs, and ideology

dc.authorid Alper, Sinan/0000-0002-9051-0690
dc.authorid Bayrak, Fatih/0000-0001-6350-6234
dc.contributor.author Doğruyol, Burak
dc.contributor.author Yılmaz, Onurcan
dc.contributor.author Alper, Sinan
dc.contributor.author Yilmaz, Onurcan
dc.contributor.other Psychology
dc.date.accessioned 2024-06-23T21:36:55Z
dc.date.available 2024-06-23T21:36:55Z
dc.date.issued 2023
dc.department Kadir Has University en_US
dc.department-temp [Bayrak, Fatih] Baskent Univ, Dept Psychol, Ankara, Turkiye; [Dogruyol, Burak; Yilmaz, Onurcan] Kadir Has Univ, Dept Psychol, Istanbul, Turkiye; [Alper, Sinan] Yasar Univ, Dept Psychol, Izmir, Turkiye en_US
dc.description Alper, Sinan/0000-0002-9051-0690; Bayrak, Fatih/0000-0001-6350-6234 en_US
dc.description.abstract Literature highlights the distinction between intuitive and analytic thinking as a prominent cognitive style distinction, leading to the proposal of various theories within the framework of the dual process model. However, it remains unclear whether individuals differ in their thinking styles along a single dimension, from intuitive to analytic, or if other dimensions are at play. Moreover, the presence of numerous thinking style measures, employing different terminology but conceptually overlapping, leads to confusion. To address these complexities, Newton et al. suggested the idea that individuals vary across multiple dimensions of intuitive-analytic thinking styles and distinguished thinking styles between 4 distinct types: Actively open-minded thinking, close-minded thinking, preference for effortful thinking, and preference for intuitive thinking. They proposed a new measure for this 4-factor disposition, The 4-Component Thinking Styles Questionnaire (4-CTSQ), to comprehensively capture the psychological outcomes related to thinking styles; however, no independent test exists. In the current pre-registered studies, we test the validity of 4-CTSQ for the first time beyond the original study and examine the association of the proposed measure with various factors, including morality, conspiracy beliefs, paranormal and religious beliefs, vaccine hesitancy, and ideology in an underrepresented culture, Turkiye. We found that the correlated 4-factor model of 4-CTSQ is an appropriate measure to capture individual differences based on cognitive style. The results endorse the notion that cognitive style differences are characterized by distinct structures rather than being confined to two ends of a single continuum. en_US
dc.identifier.citationcount 0
dc.identifier.doi 10.1017/jdm.2023.45
dc.identifier.issn 1930-2975
dc.identifier.scopusquality Q1
dc.identifier.uri https://doi.org/10.1017/jdm.2023.45
dc.identifier.uri https://hdl.handle.net/20.500.12469/5669
dc.identifier.volume 18 en_US
dc.identifier.wos WOS:001126230400001
dc.identifier.wosquality Q2
dc.language.iso en en_US
dc.publisher Cambridge Univ Press en_US
dc.relation.publicationcategory Makale - Uluslararası Hakemli Dergi - Kurum Öğretim Elemanı en_US
dc.rights info:eu-repo/semantics/openAccess en_US
dc.subject intuitive thinking en_US
dc.subject analytic thinking en_US
dc.subject reflection en_US
dc.subject intuition en_US
dc.subject dual process model en_US
dc.subject cognitive style en_US
dc.subject morality en_US
dc.subject epistemically suspect beliefs en_US
dc.subject ideology en_US
dc.title Multidimensional intuitive-analytic thinking style and its relation to moral concerns, epistemically suspect beliefs, and ideology en_US
dc.type Article en_US
dc.wos.citedbyCount 3
dspace.entity.type Publication
relation.isAuthorOfPublication 3ee2e3d3-0646-4b7b-ae07-0876605be9bd
relation.isAuthorOfPublication 9871d16b-164e-4f1d-b0e5-8eef999e6b38
relation.isAuthorOfPublication.latestForDiscovery 3ee2e3d3-0646-4b7b-ae07-0876605be9bd
relation.isOrgUnitOfPublication 9390486a-b1dc-46cf-ad5f-31415f0c8b95
relation.isOrgUnitOfPublication.latestForDiscovery 9390486a-b1dc-46cf-ad5f-31415f0c8b95

Files